Replication Crisis

From FreeWiki

Jump to: navigation, search
The following paragraph or article is based on the Wikipedia article „Replication Crisis“ from Wikipedia, as read on 31.8.2019, under the licence of Creative Commons CC-BY-SA 3.0 Unported (short version). A list of the authors is available on the respective page of Wikipedia. Changes are possible and probable.

This article is closely connected to the article on the Decline Effect, which is part of this crisis. For further explanation and material please see also there.

The replication crisis (or replicability crisis or reproducibility crisis) is an ongoing (2019) methodological crisis in which it has been found that many scientific studies are difficult or impossible to replicate or reproduce. The replication crisis affects the social and life sciences most severely.[1][2] The crisis has long-standing roots; the phrase was coined in the early 2010s[3] as part of a growing awareness of the problem. The replication crisis represents an important body of research in metascience.[4]

Because the reproducibility of experiments is an essential part of the scientific method,[5] the inability to replicate the studies of others has potentially grave consequences for many fields of science in which significant theories are grounded on unreproducible experimental work. The replication crisis has been particularly widely discussed in the field of psychology (and in particular, social psychology) and in medicine, where a number of efforts have been made to re-investigate classic results, and to attempt to determine both the reliability of the results, and, if found to be unreliable, the reasons for the failure of replication.[6][7]

Scope of the crisis

Overall

According to a 2016 poll of 1,500 scientists reported that 70% of them had failed to reproduce at least one other scientist's experiment (50% had failed to reproduce one of their own experiments).[8] In 2009, 2% of scientists admitted to falsifying studies at least once and 14% admitted to personally knowing someone who did. Misconducts were reported more frequently by medical researchers than others.[9]

Psychology

Several factors have combined to put psychology at the center of the controversy.[10] Much of the focus has been on the area of social psychology,[11] although other areas of psychology such as clinical psychology,[12][13] developmental psychology,[14] and educational research have also been implicated.[15][16] According to a 2018 survey of 200 meta-analyses, "psychological research is, on average, afflicted with low statistical power".[17]

Firstly, questionable research practices (QRPs) have been identified as common in the field.[18] Such practices, while not intentionally fraudulent, involve capitalizing on the gray area of acceptable scientific practices or exploiting flexibility in data collection, analysis, and reporting, often in an effort to obtain a desired outcome. Examples of QRPs include selective reporting or partial publication of data (reporting only some of the study conditions or collected dependent measures in a publication), optional stopping (choosing when to stop data collection, often based on statistical significance of tests), p-value rounding (rounding p-values down to 0.05 to suggest statistical significance), file drawer effect (nonpublication of data), post-hoc storytelling (framing exploratory analyses as confirmatory analyses), and manipulation of outliers (either removing outliers or leaving outliers in a dataset to cause a statistical test to be significant).[18][19][20][21] A survey of over 2,000 psychologists indicated that a majority of respondents admitted to using at least one QRP.[18] False positive conclusions, often resulting from the pressure to publish or the author's own confirmation bias, are an inherent hazard in the field, requiring a certain degree of skepticism on the part of readers.[22]

Secondly, psychology and social psychology in particular, has found itself at the center of several scandals involving outright fraudulent research, most notably the admitted data fabrication by Diederik Stapel[23] as well as allegations against others. However, most scholars acknowledge that fraud is, perhaps, the lesser contribution to replication crises.

Thirdly, several effects in psychological science have been found to be difficult to replicate even before the current replication crisis. For example, the scientific journal Judgment and Decision Making has published several studies over the years that fail to provide support for the unconscious thought theory. Replications appear particularly difficult when research trials are pre-registered and conducted by research groups not highly invested in the theory under questioning.

These three elements together have resulted in renewed attention for replication supported by psychologist Daniel Kahneman.[24] Scrutiny of many effects have shown that several core beliefs are hard to replicate. A recent special edition of the journal Social Psychology focused on replication studies and a number of previously held beliefs were found to be difficult to replicate.[25] A 2012 special edition of the journal Perspectives on Psychological Science also focused on issues ranging from publication bias to null-aversion that contribute to the replication crises in psychology.[26] In 2015, the first open empirical study of reproducibility in Psychology was published, called the Reproducibility Project. Researchers from around the world collaborated to replicate 100 empirical studies from three top Psychology journals. Fewer than half of the attempted replications were successful at producing statistically significant results in the expected directions, though most of the attempted replications did produce trends in the expected directions.[27]

Many research trials and meta-analyses are compromised by poor quality and conflicts of interest that involve both authors and professional advocacy organizations, resulting in many false positives regarding the effectiveness of certain types of psychotherapy.[28]

Although the British newspaper The Independent wrote that the results of the reproducibility project show that much of the published research is just "psycho-babble",[29] the replication crisis does not necessarily mean that psychology is unscientific.[30][31][32] Rather this process is a healthy if sometimes acrimonious part of the scientific process in which old ideas or those that cannot withstand careful scrutiny are pruned,[33][34] although this pruning process is not always effective.[35][36] The consequence is that some areas of psychology once considered solid, such as social priming, have come under increased scrutiny due to failed replications.[37]

Nobel laureate and professor emeritus in psychology Daniel Kahneman argued that the original authors should be involved in the replication effort because the published methods are often too vague.[38] Others such as Dr. Andrew Wilson disagree and argue that the methods should be written down in detail.[38] An investigation of replication rates in psychology in 2012 indicated higher success rates of replication in replication studies when there was author overlap with the original authors of a study[39] (91.7% successful replication rates in studies with author overlap compared to 64.6% success replication rates without author overlap).

Psychology replication rates

A report by the Open Science Collaboration in August 2015 that was coordinated by Brian Nosek estimated the reproducibility of 100 studies in psychological science from three high-ranking psychology journals.[40] Overall, 36% of the replications yielded significant findings (p value below 0.05) compared to 97% of the original studies that had significant effects. The mean effect size in the replications was approximately half the magnitude of the effects reported in the original studies.

The same paper examined the reproducibility rates and effect sizes by journal (Journal of Personality and Social Psychology [JPSP], Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition [JEP:LMC], Psychological Science [PSCI]) and discipline (social psychology, cognitive psychology). Study replication rates were 23% for JPSP, 38% for JEP:LMC, and 38% for PSCI. Studies in the field of cognitive psychology had a higher replication rate (50%) than studies in the field of social psychology (25%).

An analysis of the publication history in the top 100 psychology journals between 1900 and 2012 indicated that approximately 1.6% of all psychology publications were replication attempts.[39] Articles were considered a replication attempt if the term "replication" appeared in the text. A subset of those studies (500 studies) was randomly selected for further examination and yielded a lower replication rate of 1.07% (342 of the 500 studies [68.4%] were actually replications). In the subset of 500 studies, analysis indicated that 78.9% of published replication attempts were successful. The rate of successful replication was significantly higher when at least one author of the original study was part of the replication attempt (91.7% relative to 64.6%).

A study published in 2018 in Nature Human Behaviour sought to replicate 21 social and behavioral science papers from Nature and Science, finding that only 13 could be successfully replicated.[41][42] Similarly, in a study conducted under the auspices of the Center for Open Science, a team of 186 researchers from 60 different laboratories (representing 36 different nationalities from 6 different continents) conducted replications of 28 classic and contemporary findings in psychology.[43] The focus of the study was not only on whether or not the findings from the original papers replicated, but also on the extent to which findings varied as a function of variations in samples and contexts. Overall, 14 of the 28 findings failed to replicate despite massive sample sizes. However, if a finding replicated, it replicated in most samples, while if a finding was not replicated, it failed to replicate with little variation across samples and contexts. This evidence is inconsistent with a popular explanation that failures to replicate in psychology are likely due to changes in the sample between the original and replication study.[44]

A disciplinary social dilemma

Highlighting the social structure that discourages replication in psychology, Brian D. Earp and Jim A. C. Everett enumerated five points as to why replication attempts are uncommon:[45][46]

  1. "Independent, direct replications of others' findings can be time-consuming for the replicating researcher
  2. "[Replications] are likely to take energy and resources directly away from other projects that reflect one's own original thinking
  3. "[Replications] are generally harder to publish (in large part because they are viewed as being unoriginal)
  4. "Even if [replications] are published, they are likely to be seen as 'bricklaying' exercises, rather than as major contributions to the field
  5. "[Replications] bring less recognition and reward, and even basic career security, to their authors"[47]

For these reasons the authors advocated that psychology is facing a disciplinary social dilemma, where the interests of the discipline are at odds with the interests of the individual researcher.

Medicine

Out of 49 medical studies from 1990–2003, with more than 1000 citations, 45 claimed that studied therapy was effective. Out of these studies, 16% were contradicted by subsequent studies, 16% had found stronger effects than did subsequent studies, 44% were replicated, and 24% remained largely unchallenged.[48] The US Food and Drug Administration in 1977–1990 found flaws in 10–20% of medical studies.[49] In a paper published in 2012, Glenn Begley, a biotech consultant working at Amgen, and Lee Ellis, at the University of Texas, argued that only 11% of the pre-clinical cancer studies could be replicated.[50][51]

A 2016 article by John Ioannidis, Professor of Medicine and of Health Research and Policy at Stanford University School of Medicine and a Professor of Statistics at Stanford University School of Humanities and Sciences, elaborated on "Why Most Clinical Research Is Not Useful".[52] In the article Ioannidis laid out some of the problems and called for reform, characterizing certain points for medical research to be useful again; one example he made was the need for medicine to be "patient centered" (e.g. in the form of the Patient-Centered Outcomes Research Institute) instead of the current practice to mainly take care of "the needs of physicians, investigators, or sponsors". Ioannidis is known for his research focus on science itself since the 2005 paper "Why Most Published Research Findings Are False".[53]

Marketing

Marketing is another discipline with a "desperate need" for replication.[54] Many famous marketing studies fail to be repeated upon replication, a notable example being the "too-many-choices" effect, in which a high number of choices of product makes a consumer less likely to purchase.[55] In addition to the previously mentioned arguments, replications studies in marketing are needed to examine the applicability of theories and models across countries and cultures, which is especially important because of possible influences of globalization.[56]

Economics

A 2016 study in the journal Science found that one-third of 18 experimental studies from two top-tier economics journals (American Economic Review and the Quarterly Journal of Economics) failed to be successfully replicated.[57][58] A 2017 study in the Economic Journal suggested that "the majority of the average effects in the empirical economics literature are exaggerated by a factor of at least 2 and at least one-third are exaggerated by a factor of 4 or more".[59]

Sports Science

A 2018 study took the field of exercise and sports science to task for insufficient replication studies, limited reporting of null results and trivial results, and insufficient research transparency.[60] Statisticians have criticized sports science for common use of a controversial statistical method called "magnitude-based inference" that has allowed sports scientists to extract apparently significant results from noisy data where ordinary hypothesis testing would have found none.[61]

Hydrology

A 2019 study in Scientific Data found that a small number of articles in hydrology and water resources journals could be reproduced due to data unavailability. The study "estimated, with 95% confidence, that results might be reproduced for only 0.6% to 6.8% of all 1,989 articles".[62][63]Cite error: Closing </ref> missing for <ref> tag

Natural constants

One of the pillars of physics and all sciences based on the laws described in physics is the constancy of the natural constants. This constancy is a general assumption because it can principally not been proven. One can only state that they have been constant since humans are able to measure them, which is an extremely short time span compared to the age of the universe.
Nevertheless there is good reason to doubt even this basic assumption of the physical sciences.„At the end of 1998 the CODATA even decided to increase the uncertainty of the accepted value for the gravitational constant from 128 ppm to 1500 ppm. This remarkable step of increasing the uncertainty instead of decreasing was made to reflect the discrepancies between recent experiments, which span a wide range of more than 0.7 %.”[64]
And in his work “The Science Delusion – Freeing the Spirit of Enquiry” biologist and philosopher Rupert Sheldrake[65] discusses this problem and gives lots of examples showing that the fundamental constants may not be as eternal as was thought but object to change by time. He prefers to see the “constants” more as habits of nature than as eternal laws.

Causes of the crisis

Bad Science

The Sokal affair and the discussion in its wake has shown clearly that there is a major problem of not distinguishing between real and fake science.[66] The Sokal affair and the discussion in its wake has shown clearly that there is a major problem of not distinguishing between real and fake science.[67]
The editor of the prominent medical journal Lancet[68] Richard Horton writes about “apparent endemicity of bad research behaviour” and “bad scientific practices” when he points out that “much of the scientific literature, perhaps half, may simply be untrue.” As the editor of the most important medical journals he is in a position to recognize the problem.

High rate of publications

In fact some predictions of a possible crisis in the quality control mechanism of science can be traced back several decades, especially among scholars in science and technology studies (STS). Derek de Solla Price – considered the father of scientometrics – predicted that science could reach 'senility' as a result of its own exponential growth.[69] Some present day literature seems to vindicate this 'overflow' prophesy, lamenting at decay in both attention and quality.[70][71]

Philosopher and historian of science Jerome R. Ravetz predicted in his 1971 book Scientific Knowledge and Its Social Problems that science – in moving from the little science made of restricted communities of scientists to big science or techno-science – would suffer major problems in its internal system of quality control. Ravetz anticipated that modern science's system of rewarding scientists for research might become dysfunctional, the present 'publish or perish' challenge, creating perverse incentives to publish any findings however dubious. For Ravetz quality in science is maintained when there is a community of scholars linked by norms and standards, and a willingness to stand by these.

Historian Philip Mirowski offered more recently a similar diagnosis in his 2011 book Science Mart (2011).[72] 'Mart' is here a reference to the retail giant 'Walmart' and an allusion to the commodification of science. In the analysis of Mirowski, when science becomes a commodity being traded in a market, its quality collapses. Mirowski argues his case by tracing the decay of science to the decision of major corporations to close their in-house laboratories in order to outsource their work to universities, and subsequently to move their research away from universities to even cheaper contract research organizations (CRO).

Insufficient control

The crisis of science's quality control system is affecting the use of science for policy. This is the thesis of a recent work by a group of STS scholars, who identify in 'evidence based (or informed) policy' a point of present tension.[73][74][75][76] Economist Noah Smith suggests that a factor in the crisis has been the overvaluing of research in academia and undervaluing of teaching ability, especially in fields with few major recent discoveries.[77]In the journal Science article “Who’s Afraid of Peer Review?”[78] author John Bohannon explains the problem of the growing number of open access journals and there lack of effective peer review. Of 300 fake articles sent out to as many different online journals more than half were accepted without relevant criticism.

Publication bias

Another major cause is publication bias, the fact that positive results are more likely to get published than negative (or null) results. This may lead to the canonization of false facts [79].

Fundamental causes in the scientific paradigm

All the above reasons for lack of replicability have one thing in common: They are consequences of human fault, misconduct or mistakes and could theoretically be corrected by discipline, correctness and better control. Nevertheless there remains the possibility that – apart from all those considerations and observation being correct – there could also be more fundamental problems or the axiom of replicability and the constancy of nature as such.
The biologist, philosopher and meta-scientist Rupert Sheldrake has pointed out that there is ample reason to doubt the belief in the full replicability and reliability of the hard sciences as well and that there might be a problem with the assumption of replicability as such.[80]

In any case, the Decline effect (see main article) indicates general problems of replicability because it cannot be fully explained by the causes proposed so far. It was discovered by J.B.Rhine in the 20s of the last century, long before the current crisis and under completely different circumstances. Rhine found the effect in his own research, even in areas whose statistical validity he himself had previously verified.

Public Response

Political Repercussions

In the US, science's reproducibility crisis has become a topic of political contention, linked to the attempt to diminish regulations – e.g. of emissions of pollutants, with the argument that these regulations are based on non-reproducible science.[81][76] Previous attempts with the same aim accused studies of being non-transparent.[82]

Public discussion

Apart from studies and articles in scientific journals there has been little public discussion of the Replicability Crisis as yet and few reports in the mass media. Most of the discussions are related to statistical, economical and sociological reasons only.[83]
The question whether there is a general problem or replicability behind these findings has not been tackled so far. And there has been very little effort in the hard sciences to deal with the replication issue as there seems to be a consense that the problem is only one of the social and life sciences.

Addressing the replication crisis

Replication has been referred to as "the cornerstone of science".[84][85] Replication studies attempt to evaluate whether published results reflect true findings or false positives. The integrity of scientific findings and reproducibility of research are important as they form the knowledge foundation on which future studies are built.

Metascience

Metascience is the use of scientific methodology to study science itself. Metascience seeks to increase the quality of scientific research while reducing waste. It is also known as "research on research" and "the science of science", as it uses research methods to study how research is done and where improvements can be made. Metascience concerns itself with all fields of research and has been described as "a bird's eye view of science."[86] In the words of John Ioannidis, "Science is the best thing that has happened to human beings ... but we can do it better."[87]

Meta-research continues to be conducted to identify the roots of the crisis and to address them. Methods of addressing the crisis include pre-registration of scientific studies and clinical trials as well as the founding of organizations such as CONSORT and the EQUATOR Network that issue guidelines for methodology and reporting. There are continuing efforts to reform the system of academic incentives, to improve the peer review process, to reduce the misuse of statistics, to combat bias in scientific literature, and to increase the overall quality and efficiency of the scientific process.

Tackling publication bias with pre-registration of studies

A recent innovation in scientific publishing to address the replication crisis is through the use of registered reports.[88][89] The registered report format requires authors to submit a description of the study methods and analyses prior to data collection. Once the method and analysis plan is vetted through peer-review, publication of the findings is provisionally guaranteed, based on whether the authors follow the proposed protocol. One goal of registered reports is to circumvent the publication bias toward significant findings that can lead to implementation of questionable research practices and to encourage publication of studies with rigorous methods.

The journal Psychological Science has encouraged the preregistration of studies and the reporting of effect sizes and confidence intervals.[90] The editor in chief also noted that the editorial staff will be asking for replication of studies with surprising findings from examinations using small sample sizes before allowing the manuscripts to be published.

Moreover, only a very small proportion of academic journals in psychology and neurosciences explicitly stated that they welcome submissions of replication studies in their aim and scope or instructions to authors.[91][92] This phenomenon does not encourage the reporting or even attempt on replication studies.

Emphasizing replication attempts in teaching

Based on coursework in experimental methods at MIT and Stanford, it has been suggested that methods courses in psychology emphasize replication attempts rather than original studies.[93][94] Such an approach would help students learn scientific methodology and provide numerous independent replications of meaningful scientific findings that would test the replicability of scientific findings. Some have recommended that graduate students should be required to publish a high-quality replication attempt on a topic related to their doctoral research prior to graduation.[46]

Reducing the p-value required for claiming significance of new results

Many publications require a p-value of p < 0.05 to claim statistical significance. The paper "Redefine statistical significance",[95] signed by a large number of scientists and mathematicians, proposes that in "fields where the threshold for defining statistical significance for new discoveries is P < 0.05, we propose a change to P < 0.005. This simple step would immediately improve the reproducibility of scientific research in many fields."

Their rationale is that "a leading cause of non-reproducibility (is that the) statistical standards of evidence for claiming new discoveries in many fields of science are simply too low. Associating 'statistically significant' findings with P < 0.05 results in a high rate of false positives even in the absence of other experimental, procedural and reporting problems."

Addressing the misinterpretation of p-values

Although statisticians are unanimous that use of the p < 0.05 provides weaker evidence than is generally appreciated, there is a lack of unanimity about what should be done about it. Some have advocated that Bayesian methods should replace p-values. This has not happened on a wide scale, partly because it is complicated, and partly because many users distrust the specification of prior distributions in the absence of hard data. A simplified version of the Bayesian argument, based on testing a point null hypothesis was suggested by Colquhoun (2014, 2017).[96][97] The logical problems of inductive inference were discussed in "The problem with p-values" (2016).[98]

The hazards of reliance on p-values were emphasized by pointing out that even observation of p = 0.001 was not necessarily strong evidence against the null hypothesis.[97] Despite the fact that the likelihood ratio in favour of the alternative hypothesis over the null is close to 100, if the hypothesis was implausible, with a prior probability of a real effect being 0.1, even the observation of p = 0.001 would have a false positive risk of 8 percent. It would not even reach the 5 percent level.

It was recommended[97] that the terms "significant" and "non-significant" should not be used. p-values and confidence intervals should still be specified, but they should be accompanied by an indication of the false positive risk. It was suggested that the best way to do this is to calculate the prior probability that would be necessary to believe in order to achieve a false positive risk of, say, 5%. The calculations can be done with R scripts that are provided,[97] or, more simply, with a web calculator.[99] This so-called reverse Bayesian approach, which was suggested by Matthews (2001),[100] is one way to avoid the problem that the prior probability is rarely known.

Encouraging larger sample sizes

To improve the quality of replications, larger sample sizes than those used in the original study are often needed.[101] Larger sample sizes are needed because estimates of effect sizes in published work are often exaggerated due to publication bias and large sampling variability associated with small sample sizes in an original study.[102][103][103][104] Further, using significance thresholds usually leads to inflated effects, because particularly with small sample sizes, only the largest effects will become significant.[105]

Sharing raw data in online repositories

Online repositories where data, protocols, and findings can be stored and evaluated by the public seek to improve the integrity and reproducibility of research. Examples of such repositories include the Open Science Framework, Registry of Research Data Repositories, and Psychfiledrawer.org. Sites like Open Science Framework offer badges for using open science practices in an effort to incentivize scientists. However, there has been concern that those who are most likely to provide their data and code for analyses are the researchers that are likely the most sophisticated.[106] John Ioannidis at Stanford University suggested that "the paradox may arise that the most meticulous and sophisticated and method-savvy and careful researchers may become more susceptible to criticism and reputation attacks by reanalyzers who hunt for errors, no matter how negligible these errors are".[106]

Funding for replication studies

In July 2016 the Netherlands Organisation for Scientific Research made €3 million available for replication studies. The funding is for replication based on reanalysis of existing data and replication by collecting and analysing new data. Funding is available in the areas of social sciences, health research and healthcare innovation.[107]

In 2013 the Laura and John Arnold Foundation funded the launch of The Center for Open Science with a $5.25 million grant and by 2017 had provided an additional $10 million in funding.[108] It also funded the launch of the Meta-Research Innovation Center at Stanford at Stanford University run by John Ioannidis and Steven Goodman to study ways to improve scientific research.[108] It also provided funding for the AllTrials initiative led in part by Ben Goldacre.[108]

Emphasize triangulation, not just replication

Marcus R. Munafò and George Davey Smith argue, in a piece published by Nature, that research should emphasize triangulation, not just replication. They claim that,

replication alone will get us only so far (and) might actually make matters worse ... We believe that an essential protection against flawed ideas is triangulation. This is the strategic use of multiple approaches to address one question. Each approach has its own unrelated assumptions, strengths and weaknesses. Results that agree across different methodologies are less likely to be artefacts. ... Maybe one reason replication has captured so much interest is the often-repeated idea that falsification is at the heart of the scientific enterprise. This idea was popularized by Karl Popper's 1950s maxim that theories can never be proved, only falsified. Yet an overemphasis on repeating experiments could provide an unfounded sense of certainty about findings that rely on a single approach. ... philosophers of science have moved on since Popper. Better descriptions of how scientists actually work include what epistemologist Peter Lipton called in 1991 "inference to the best explanation".[106]

References

  1. Script error: No such module "Citation/CS1".
  2. Why 'Statistical Significance' Is Often Insignificant. Retrieved on 7 November 2017.
  3. Script error: No such module "Citation/CS1".
  4. Reproducibility of Scientific Results. Metaphysics Research Lab, Stanford University (2018). Retrieved on 19 May 2019.
  5. Staddon, John (2017) Scientific Method: How science works, fails to work or pretends to work. Taylor and Francis.
  6. Gary Marcus (May 1, 2013). The Crisis in Social Psychology That Isn't. The New Yorker.
  7. Jonah Lehrer (December 13, 2010). The Truth Wears Off. The New Yorker.
  8. Is There a Reproducibility Crisis in Science?. Nature Video, Scientific American (28 May 2016). Retrieved on 15 August 2019.
  9. Script error: No such module "Citation/CS1".
  10. No, science's reproducibility problem is not limited to psychology. Retrieved on 10 September 2015.
  11. Script error: No such module "citation/CS1".
  12. Script error: No such module "Citation/CS1".
  13. Script error: No such module "Citation/CS1".
  14. Script error: No such module "Citation/CS1".
  15. Script error: No such module "Citation/CS1".
  16. Script error: No such module "Citation/CS1".
  17. Script error: No such module "Citation/CS1".
  18. 18.0 18.1 18.2 Script error: No such module "Citation/CS1".
  19. Script error: No such module "Citation/CS1".
  20. Research misconduct - The grey area of Questionable Research Practices. Retrieved on 2015-11-13.
  21. Script error: No such module "Citation/CS1".
  22. Script error: No such module "Citation/CS1".
  23. Script error: No such module "Citation/CS1".
  24. Kahneman, Daniel. A New Etiquette for Replication.
  25. Script error: No such module "Citation/CS1".
  26. Table of Contents
  27. Script error: No such module "Citation/CS1".
  28. Coyne, James (April 15, 2014). Are meta analyses conducted by professional organizations more trustworthy?. Mind the Brain. Retrieved on September 13, 2016.
  29. Script error: No such module "citation/CS1".
  30. Why Psychologists' Food Fight Matters (31 July 2014).
  31. Psychology Is Starting To Deal With Its Replication Problem (27 August 2015).
  32. Science Isn't Broken (19 August 2015).
  33. Script error: No such module "citation/CS1".
  34. Script error: No such module "Citation/CS1".
  35. Script error: No such module "Citation/CS1".
  36. Script error: No such module "Citation/CS1".
  37. Script error: No such module "Citation/CS1".
  38. 38.0 38.1 Script error: No such module "citation/CS1".
  39. 39.0 39.1 Script error: No such module "Citation/CS1".
  40. Script error: No such module "Citation/CS1".
  41. Script error: No such module "citation/CS1".
  42. Script error: No such module "Citation/CS1".
  43. Script error: No such module "Citation/CS1".
  44. Script error: No such module "Citation/CS1".
  45. See also Earp and Trafimow, 2015
  46. 46.0 46.1 Script error: No such module "Citation/CS1".
  47. Resolving the replication crisis in social psychology? A new proposal. Society for Personality and Social Psychology. Retrieved on 2015-11-18.
  48. Script error: No such module "Citation/CS1".
  49. Script error: No such module "Citation/CS1".
  50. Script error: No such module "Citation/CS1".
  51. Script error: No such module "Citation/CS1".
  52. Script error: No such module "Citation/CS1".
  53. Script error: No such module "Citation/CS1".
  54. Script error: No such module "Citation/CS1".
  55. Script error: No such module "Citation/CS1".
  56. Script error: No such module "Citation/CS1".
  57. Script error: No such module "Citation/CS1".
  58. Script error: No such module "citation/CS1".
  59. Script error: No such module "Citation/CS1".
  60. Script error: No such module "Citation/CS1".
  61. Script error: No such module "citation/CS1".
  62. Script error: No such module "Citation/CS1".
  63. https://www.nature.com/articles/sdata201930#f2
  64. Ulf Kleinevoß: Bestimmung der Newtonschen Gravitationskonstanten, Dissertation Januar 2002, Wuppertal, S.1, Abstract; [1]
  65. Rupert Sheldrake: The Science Delusion – Freeing the Spirit of Enquiry; London 2012, Hodder& Stoughton, ISBN 978 1 444 72795 1. Chapter 3: Are the Laws of Nature Fixed?
  66. Peter Boghossian, Ed.D. (aka Peter Boyle, Ed.D.), James Lindsay, Ph.D. (aka, Jamie Lindsay, Ph.D.): The Conceptual Penis as a Social Construct: A Sokal-Style Hoax on Gender Studies. SKEPTIC, 19.05.2017.[2]; Alexander Durin: Fehler im System mancher Wissenschaften. Telepolis, Heise, 02.03.2014. [3]; Alan D. Sokal: Transgressing the Boundaries: Towards a Transformative Hermeneutics of Quantum Gravity. Social Text 46/47:217-252, 1996.... le.html.
  67. Peter Boghossian, Ed.D. (aka Peter Boyle, Ed.D.), James Lindsay, Ph.D. (aka, Jamie Lindsay, Ph.D.): The Conceptual Penis as a Social Construct: A Sokal-Style Hoax on Gender Studies. SKEPTIC, 19.05.2017.[4]; Alexander Durin: Fehler im System mancher Wissenschaften. Telepolis, Heise, 02.03.2014. [5]; Alan D. Sokal: Transgressing the Boundaries: Towards a Transformative Hermeneutics of Quantum Gravity. Social Text 46/47:217-252, 1996.... le.html.
  68. Richard Horton – Offline: What is medicine’s 5 sigma? In: The Lancet VOLUME 385, ISSUE 9976, P1380, APRIL 11, 2015; [6]
  69. Script error: No such module "citation/CS1".
  70. Script error: No such module "Citation/CS1".
  71. Script error: No such module "Citation/CS1".
  72. Script error: No such module "citation/CS1".
  73. Script error: No such module "Citation/CS1".
  74. Script error: No such module "citation/CS1".
  75. Script error: No such module "Citation/CS1".
  76. 76.0 76.1 Script error: No such module "Citation/CS1".
  77. Academic signaling and the post-truth world. Stony Brook University (2016-12-14). Retrieved on 5 November 2017.
  78. John Bohannon: Who’s Afraid of Peer Review?, Science 04 Oct 2013: Vol. 342, Issue 6154, pp. 60-65; DOI: 10.1126/science.342.6154.60
  79. Script error: No such module "Citation/CS1".
  80. Rupert Sheldrake: How the Universal Gravitational Constant Varies; The Replicability Crisis in Science; Science Set Free: 10 Paths to New Discovery, ISBN-13: 978-0770436704, Publ. Deepak Chopra; 1st ed. (4. September 2012); The Science Delusion, ISBN-13: 978-1444727944, Publisher: Coronet (6 Dec. 2012)
  81. Script error: No such module "Citation/CS1".
  82. Script error: No such module "citation/CS1".
  83. See all of the cited literature.
  84. Script error: No such module "Citation/CS1".
  85. Script error: No such module "Citation/CS1".
  86. Script error: No such module "Citation/CS1".
  87. On communicating science and uncertainty: A podcast with John Ioannidis (8 December 2015). Retrieved on 20 May 2019.
  88. Registered Replication Reports. Association for Psychological Science. Retrieved on 2015-11-13.
  89. Script error: No such module "citation/CS1".
  90. Script error: No such module "Citation/CS1".
  91. Script error: No such module "Citation/CS1".
  92. Script error: No such module "Citation/CS1".
  93. Script error: No such module "Citation/CS1".
  94. Script error: No such module "Citation/CS1".
  95. Script error: No such module "Citation/CS1".
  96. Script error: No such module "Citation/CS1".
  97. 97.0 97.1 97.2 97.3 Script error: No such module "Citation/CS1".
  98. The problem with p-values. Aeon Magazine. Retrieved on 11 December 2016.
  99. Calculator for false positive risk (FPR). UCL.
  100. Script error: No such module "Citation/CS1".
  101. {{Cite journal|title = Is psychology suffering from a replication crisis? What does
  102. Script error: No such module "Citation/CS1".
  103. 103.0 103.1 Script error: No such module "Citation/CS1".
  104. Script error: No such module "Citation/CS1".
  105. Script error: No such module "Citation/CS1".
  106. 106.0 106.1 106.2 Script error: No such module "Citation/CS1".
  107. NWO makes 3 million available for Replication Studies pilot. Retrieved on 2 August 2016.
  108. 108.0 108.1 108.2 Script error: No such module "citation/CS1".

Further reading